Turnitin Originality Report
- Processed on: 27-Sep-2011 6:08 AM HKT
- ID: 204148789
- Word Count: 1435
- Submitted: 1
index.html By Jayant Jayant
- Internet Sources:
- 21%
- Publications:
- 41%
- Student Papers:
- 8%
This is a preview of the print version of your report. Please click "print" to continue or "done" to close this window.
done23% match (publications)
4% match (Internet from 2/1/10)
http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdf
2% match (Internet from 5/5/09)
http://psychology.stanford.edu/~jlm/pdfs/CaramazzaMahon03.pdf
2% match (publications)
2% match (publications)
1% match (Internet from 4/13/10)
http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences/fulltext/S1364-6613(06)00302-0?large_figure=true
1% match (Internet from 5/17/11)
http://www.visionlab.harvard.edu/Members/Ken/Ken%20papers%20for%20web%20page/165WilmerResponse.pdf
1% match (publications)
1% match (Internet from 2/2/09)
http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2008_Almeida_et_al.pdf
1% match (Internet from 3/21/10)
http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2003_Capitani_et_al.pdf
1% match (student papers from 00/00/00)
1% match (Internet from 4/14/11)
1% match (Internet from 6/24/10)
http://www.emory.edu/LIVING_LINKS/pdf_attachments/dewaal_ferrari_2010.pdf
What drives the organization of object knowledge in the brain?
Paper Review It is the view in cognition that different domains of knowledge are organized in the brain and processed by distinct neural mechanisms. In the present paper, the authors suggest an extension to the currently existing domain-specific hypothesis, based on connectivity between different functional regions of the brain. They claim that domain expertise in a region is a result of
innate connectivity
between
a
group
of regions that also process information about
the same
domain.
Category specificity as a means to study
organizational
constraints
Patients with
brain-
injuries who show
category specific semantic impairments
also show
conceptual level impairments[
0]. The
categories of category-specific semantic deficits are animate objects, inanimate biological objects and artefacts[
1].
The
studies suggests that
domain-
specific constraints dictate
organization of conceptual knowledge in the brain. A
lot of research on object knowledge has focused on the ventral visual pathway which has its roots in the early visual areas. It plays a primary role in object recognition and classification. The ventral region exhibits spatial biases according to semantic distinctions, i.e., to say that it differentially processes objects belonging to different categories, eg: faces, tools, body parts, words, fruits etc. Such bias raises questions as to what factors encourage such differential processing in the ventral region. The thrust of the majority of research on the ventral region has been on stimulus driven response. Coming to categorical distinctions, this approach doesnt seem valid in light of the fact that response depends on more than just the primary input, often drawing upon prior knowledge. A classic example here would be the Stroop Test. Herein, the authors lay their core idea: that what the ventral stream does after object recognition depends entirely on how it
is connected to the rest of the brain.
The paper studies visual object recognition and taxonomy, but similar ideas should also apply to object recognition via other faculties. The authors argue that such connectivity in the brain is innate in nature, and the driving factor behind semantic specialization in the ventral stream, drawing upon evidence to support their theory. graph
The Distributed Domain Specific
Theory
A domain specific neural network
in the
brain
has two primary characteristics. First is distributed processing, i.e., it consists of distinct regions in the brain each processing
a different type of information about the domain[
0]. Interesting to note, is the fact that such domains are evolutionary important domains so as to permit a dedicated processing network, good examples being the domain of faces and the domain of tools. Secondly, the set of computations over such a domain must be sufficiently erratic to motivate the formation of such a pathway. For example, social information is called for when a face is identified. Handling expertise is called upon on recognising a tool or similarly shaped objects. As a personal example, I seem to think of grasping sticks when I notice a peacock's neck. The authors propose that categorical specialization in the ventral stream is expressed as connectivity patterns
between the ventral stream and
the rest
of the brain[
0]. For example, specialization of the fusiform face area in the ventral pathway to identify faces is driven by its networking with areas that store socially relevant information.
Specificity for tools and manipulable objects in the medial fusiform gyrus is driven, in part, by connectivity between that region and regions of parietal cortex that subserve object manipulation [23-26].
Role of visual experience in shaping ventral stream organisation There has been recent evidence refuting the need for visual experience for classification of some types of data in the ventral stream. The authors themselves are credited with the finding that the same medial-to-lateral bias
that is present in sighted
people,
is
also
present in congenitally blind subjects.
But, if visual experience does not drive organization in the ventral stream, what does? Connectivity! Only through such connections in the brain, can the same areas in a congenitally blind person be activated by the same input that elicits a similar response in a sighted person. Note that this also points out the fact that connectivity is innate in nature. graph
Figure 1. Congenitally blind and sighted participants were presented with auditorily spoken words of living things (animals) and nonliving things (tools, non-manipulable objects) and were asked to make size judgments about the referents of the words. The sighted participants were also shown pictures corresponding to the same stimuli in a separate scan. For sighted participants viewing pictures, the known finding was replicated that nonliving things such as tools and large non-manipulable objects lead to differential neural responses in medial aspects of the ventral occipital-temporal cortex. This pattern of differential BOLD responses for nonliving things in medial aspects of the ventral occipital-temporal cortex was also observed in congenitally blind participants and sighted participants performing the size judgment task over auditory stimuli. These data indicate that the medial-to-lateral bias in the distribution of category-specific responses does not depend on visual experience. For details of the study, see [44].
Connectivity as an innate property: Evidence The distinguishing feature of an innate property is its similarity across individuals of a species and sometimes even cross-species similarities. Genetic variables capturing the property are a strong indication towards an innate structure. We look at the following cases: Studies of twins A couple of reports highlight a greater level of functional similarity between monozygotic twins than dizygotic twins. These studies found that in face-related tasks, the responses and performances are much
more similar in monozygotic
twins
than in dizygotic
ones
(Polk
and colleagues[50], Wilmer and colleagues[51]). Interestingly enough, such similarities are not found in
verbal and visual memory
tasks,
indicating
genetic
selectivity in
behaviours. Congenital prosopagnosia Another piece of evidence supporting
genetic contribution to
facial
recognition comes from
study of
prosopagnosia
patients, wherein patients are selectively impaired to recognise faces.
A recent study by Thomas and colleagues[55]
has
found, that
in such patients,
major white matter tracts(
responsible for connectivity)
linking the posterior occipital temporal cortex with other regions
of
the
brain are missing, thus, highlighting the importance of a network level analysis in object organisation. Another piece of evidence supporting
genetic contribution to
facial
recognition comes from
study of
prosopagnosia
patients, wherein patients are selectively impaired to recognise faces.
A recent study by Thomas and colleagues[55]
has
found, that
in such patients,
major white matter tracts(
responsible for connectivity)
linking the posterior occipital temporal cortex with other regions
of
the
brain are missing, thus, highlighting the importance of a network level analysis in object organisation. Non-human primates In functional imaging studies in monkies, and more recently in
macaques[57] and chimpanzees[58],
it was found
that
atleast
for
facial recognition, similar
clusters of face-preferring voxels
in the brain
can be found in the temporal cortex
of
monkeys, as
they do exist
in humans[
0]. It may be argued, that such patterns might exist only due to visual similarity, as is known to happed in the IT cortex. But, continued attempts to explain away this observations in terms of visual similarity have failed, often revealing tight taxonomic structures. References 0 Current paper being reviewed 1
Capitani, E. et al. (2003) What are the facts of category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 20, 213-261
11
Mahon, B.Z. and Caramazza, A. (2009) Concepts and categories: a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 1-15
23
Mahon, B.Z.
et al. (2007) Action-related properties shape object representations in the ventral stream. Neuron 55, 507-520
24
Valyear, K.F. and Culham, J.C. (2010) Observing learned object-specific functional grasps preferentially activates the ventral stream. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22,
970-984 25 Noppeney, U. et al.
(2006) Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective responses. Cereb. Cortex 16, 437-445
26
Rushworth, M.F.
S.
et al. (2006) Connection patterns distinguish 3 regions of human parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1418- 1430
44 Mahon, B.Z. et al.
(2009) Category-specific organization in the human brain does not require visual experience. Neuron 63, 397-405
50 Polk, T
.A.
et al. (2007) Nature versus nurture in ventral visual cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of twins. J. Neurosci. 27, 13921-13925
51 Wilmer, J.
et al.
(2010) Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A.
107, 5238-5241
55
Thomas,
C. et al.
(2009) Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 29-31
57
Tsao, D.Y.
et al. (2006)
A
cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science 311, 670-674
58 Parr, L
.A. et al.
(2009) Face processing
in
the chimpanzee brain. Curr. Biol. 19, 50-53