This is a preview of the print version of your report. Please click "print" to continue or "done" to close this window.

done

or Cancel

Document Viewer
 
Similarity Index
42%
What's this?
Similarity by Source
Internet Sources:
21%
Publications:
41%
Student Papers:
8%
exclude quoted exclude bibliography exclude small matches download print
mode:

Begin Match to source 12 in source list: http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/publications.htmlWhat drives the organization of object knowledge in the brain?End Match Paper Review It is the view in cognition that different domains of knowledge are organized in the brain and processed by distinct neural mechanisms. In the present paper, the authors suggest an extension to the currently existing domain-specific hypothesis, based on connectivity between different functional regions of the brain. They claim that domain expertise in a region is a result of Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. innate connectivityEnd Match between Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. aEnd Match group Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. of regions that also process information aboutEnd Match the same Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. domain.End MatchBegin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. Category specificity as a means to studyEnd Match organizational Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. constraintsEnd Match Patients with Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. brain-End Match injuries who show Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. category specific semantic impairmentsEnd Match also show Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. conceptual level impairments[End Match 0]. The Begin Match to source 10 in source list: http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2003_Capitani_et_al.pdfcategories of category-specific semantic deficits are animate objects, inanimate biological objects and artefacts[End Match 1]. Begin Match to source 10 in source list: http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2003_Capitani_et_al.pdfTheEnd Match studies suggests that Begin Match to source 10 in source list: http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2003_Capitani_et_al.pdfdomain-End Match specific constraints dictate Begin Match to source 3 in source list: http://psychology.stanford.edu/~jlm/pdfs/CaramazzaMahon03.pdforganization of conceptual knowledge in the brain. AEnd Match lot of research on object knowledge has focused on the ventral visual pathway which has its roots in the early visual areas. It plays a primary role in object recognition and classification. The ventral region exhibits spatial biases according to semantic distinctions, i.e., to say that it differentially processes objects belonging to different categories, eg: faces, tools, body parts, words, fruits etc. Such bias raises questions as to what factors encourage such differential processing in the ventral region. The thrust of the majority of research on the ventral region has been on stimulus driven response. Coming to categorical distinctions, this approach doesnt seem valid in light of the fact that response depends on more than just the primary input, often drawing upon prior knowledge. A classic example here would be the Stroop Test. Herein, the authors lay their core idea: that what the ventral stream does after object recognition depends entirely on how it Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. is connected to the rest of the brain.End Match The paper studies visual object recognition and taxonomy, but similar ideas should also apply to object recognition via other faculties. The authors argue that such connectivity in the brain is innate in nature, and the driving factor behind semantic specialization in the ventral stream, drawing upon evidence to support their theory. graph Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. The Distributed Domain SpecificEnd Match Theory Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. A domain specific neural networkEnd Match in the Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. brainEnd Match has two primary characteristics. First is distributed processing, i.e., it consists of distinct regions in the brain each processing Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. a different type of information about the domain[End Match 0]. Interesting to note, is the fact that such domains are evolutionary important domains so as to permit a dedicated processing network, good examples being the domain of faces and the domain of tools. Secondly, the set of computations over such a domain must be sufficiently erratic to motivate the formation of such a pathway. For example, social information is called for when a face is identified. Handling expertise is called upon on recognising a tool or similarly shaped objects. As a personal example, I seem to think of grasping sticks when I notice a peacock's neck. The authors propose that categorical specialization in the ventral stream is expressed as connectivity patterns Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. between the ventral stream andEnd Match the rest Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. of the brain[End Match 0]. For example, specialization of the fusiform face area in the ventral pathway to identify faces is driven by its networking with areas that store socially relevant information. Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. Specificity for tools and manipulable objects in the medial fusiform gyrus is driven, in part, by connectivity between that region and regions of parietal cortex that subserve object manipulation [23-26].End Match Role of visual experience in shaping ventral stream organisation There has been recent evidence refuting the need for visual experience for classification of some types of data in the ventral stream. The authors themselves are credited with the finding that the same medial-to-lateral bias Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. that is present in sightedEnd Match people, Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. isEnd Match also Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. present in congenitally blind subjects.End Match But, if visual experience does not drive organization in the ventral stream, what does? Connectivity! Only through such connections in the brain, can the same areas in a congenitally blind person be activated by the same input that elicits a similar response in a sighted person. Note that this also points out the fact that connectivity is innate in nature. graph Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. Figure 1. Congenitally blind and sighted participants were presented with auditorily spoken words of living things (animals) and nonliving things (tools, non-manipulable objects) and were asked to make size judgments about the referents of the words. The sighted participants were also shown pictures corresponding to the same stimuli in a separate scan. For sighted participants viewing pictures, the known finding was replicated that nonliving things such as tools and large non-manipulable objects lead to differential neural responses in medial aspects of the ventral occipital-temporal cortex. This pattern of differential BOLD responses for nonliving things in medial aspects of the ventral occipital-temporal cortex was also observed in congenitally blind participants and sighted participants performing the size judgment task over auditory stimuli. These data indicate that the medial-to-lateral bias in the distribution of category-specific responses does not depend on visual experience. For details of the study, see [44].End Match Connectivity as an innate property: Evidence The distinguishing feature of an innate property is its similarity across individuals of a species and sometimes even cross-species similarities. Genetic variables capturing the property are a strong indication towards an innate structure. We look at the following cases: Studies of twins A couple of reports highlight a greater level of functional similarity between monozygotic twins than dizygotic twins. These studies found that in face-related tasks, the responses and performances are much Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdfmore similar in monozygoticEnd Match twins Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdfthan in dizygoticEnd Match ones Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdf(PolkEnd Match and colleagues[50], Wilmer and colleagues[51]). Interestingly enough, such similarities are not found in Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. verbal and visual memoryEnd Match tasks, Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. indicatingEnd Match genetic Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. selectivity inEnd Match behaviours. Congenital prosopagnosia Another piece of evidence supporting Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. genetic contribution toEnd Match facial Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. recognition comes fromEnd Match study of Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. prosopagnosiaEnd Match patients, wherein patients are selectively impaired to recognise faces. Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. A recent study by Thomas and colleagues[55]End Match has Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. found, thatEnd Match in such patients, Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. major white matter tracts(End Match responsible for connectivity) Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. linking the posterior occipital temporal cortex with other regionsEnd Match of Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. theEnd Match brain are missing, thus, highlighting the importance of a network level analysis in object organisation. Another piece of evidence supporting Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. genetic contribution toEnd Match facial Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. recognition comes fromEnd Match study of Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. prosopagnosiaEnd Match patients, wherein patients are selectively impaired to recognise faces. Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. A recent study by Thomas and colleagues[55]End Match has Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. found, thatEnd Match in such patients, Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. major white matter tracts(End Match responsible for connectivity) Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. linking the posterior occipital temporal cortex with other regionsEnd Match of Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. theEnd Match brain are missing, thus, highlighting the importance of a network level analysis in object organisation. Non-human primates In functional imaging studies in monkies, and more recently in Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. macaques[57] and chimpanzees[58],End Match it was found Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. thatEnd Match atleast Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. forEnd Match facial recognition, similar Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. clusters of face-preferring voxelsEnd Match in the brain Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. can be found in the temporal cortexEnd Match of Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. monkeys, asEnd Match they do exist Begin Match to source 1 in source list: Mahon, B.Z.. in humans[End Match 0]. It may be argued, that such patterns might exist only due to visual similarity, as is known to happed in the IT cortex. But, continued attempts to explain away this observations in terms of visual similarity have failed, often revealing tight taxonomic structures. References 0 Current paper being reviewed 1 Begin Match to source 3 in source list: http://psychology.stanford.edu/~jlm/pdfs/CaramazzaMahon03.pdfCapitani, E. et al. (2003) What are the facts of category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 20, 213-261End Match 11 Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdfMahon, B.Z. and Caramazza, A. (2009) Concepts and categories: a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 1-15End Match 23 Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdfMahon, B.Z.End MatchBegin Match to source 9 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2008_Almeida_et_al.pdfet al. (2007) Action-related properties shape object representations in the ventral stream. Neuron 55, 507-520End Match 24 Begin Match to source 5 in source list: Paul Downing. Valyear, K.F. and Culham, J.C. (2010) Observing learned object-specific functional grasps preferentially activates the ventral stream. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22,End Match 970-984 25 Noppeney, U. et al. Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdf(2006) Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective responses. Cereb. Cortex 16, 437-445End Match 26 Begin Match to source 6 in source list: http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences/fulltext/S1364-6613(06)00302-0?large_figure=trueRushworth, M.F.End Match S. Begin Match to source 6 in source list: http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences/fulltext/S1364-6613(06)00302-0?large_figure=trueet al. (2006) Connection patterns distinguish 3 regions of human parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1418- 1430End Match 44 Mahon, B.Z. et al. Begin Match to source 8 in source list: James W. Lewis. (2009) Category-specific organization in the human brain does not require visual experience. Neuron 63, 397-405End Match 50 Polk, T Begin Match to source 8 in source list: James W. Lewis. .A.End MatchBegin Match to source 4 in source list: E. McKone. et al. (2007) Nature versus nurture in ventral visual cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of twins. J. Neurosci. 27, 13921-13925End Match 51 Wilmer, J. Begin Match to source 4 in source list: E. McKone. et al.End MatchBegin Match to source 7 in source list: http://www.visionlab.harvard.edu/Members/Ken/Ken papers for web page/165WilmerResponse.pdf(2010) Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.End Match U.S.A. Begin Match to source 7 in source list: http://www.visionlab.harvard.edu/Members/Ken/Ken papers for web page/165WilmerResponse.pdf107, 5238-5241End Match 55 Begin Match to source 7 in source list: http://www.visionlab.harvard.edu/Members/Ken/Ken papers for web page/165WilmerResponse.pdfThomas,End Match C. et al. Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdf(2009) Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 29-31End Match 57 Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdfTsao, D.Y.End Match et al. (2006) Begin Match to source 2 in source list: http://coglab.wjh.harvard.edu/~caram/PDFs/2009_Mahon_Anzellotti_Caramazza_Neuron.pdfAEnd MatchBegin Match to source 11 in source list: papercortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science 311, 670-674End Match 58 Parr, L Begin Match to source 11 in source list: paper.A. et al.End Match (2009) Face processing Begin Match to source 11 in source list: paperinEnd MatchBegin Match to source 13 in source list: http://www.emory.edu/LIVING_LINKS/pdf_attachments/dewaal_ferrari_2010.pdfthe chimpanzee brain. Curr. Biol. 19, 50-53End Match