Pasek, Kathy Hirsh; Roberta M. Golinkoff;
Action meets word: how children learn verbs
Oxford University Press, 2006, 588 pages
ISBN 0195170008, 9780199753710
topics: | cognitive-linguistics | language-acquisition | verb
Verbs are the architectural centerpiece of grammar, determining the argument structure of a sentence.Verbs can be defined syntactically or semantically. Syntactically, a verb is a word that takes a subject (or agent) or an object or both.Verbs, for example, can take different morphological forms based on gender, person, number, animacy, and indefiniteness, and they can be passivized or dativized in many languages. Semantically, verbs are words that “encode events: A cover term for states or conditions of existenceprocesses or unfoldingsand actions or executive processes” (Frawley, 1992, p. 141). A verb is a description of a relation that occurs over time.
In general, however, the first relational terms are verbs and the first verbs are motion verbs.
[There exists a debate on whether children have the syntactic category of verb (see [Pinker, 1984]) or whether they simply have the category of action word (see [Olguin & Tomasello, 1993]).]
[Gentner 1982] posits that verbs pose special challenges for word learners. Verbs label events that are comprised of components like manner (walk vs. swagger), instrument (hammer, shovel), path (ascend, descend ), and result (open, break) — any of which can be the dominant focus for the label [Talmy 1985]. Further, across languages, different components are highlighted such that manner is often conflated in English verbs (e.g., skip), while path is often an integral part of Spanish verbs (e.g., ascendere; see [Slobin 2001], or [Talmy 2000], for reviews).
Verbs also describe events in the world and events are by nature more ephemeral than the objects that nouns tend to label ([Langacker 1987]; [Slobin 2001]). Furthermore, in speech to children, verbs often label these events even before the action has taken place (Tomasello & Kruger, 1992), while nouns tend to label enduring entities available for prolonged inspection. Another difference between nouns and verbs is that nouns have a tendency to have more restricted meanings than do verbs. Finally, verbs are inherently relational; the use of a verb implies the presence of an actor to carry out that action.
p. 111-133 Image-schemas summarize spatial relations and movements in space. Note that they are not visual images (although they may be used in image construction). They are a bit like topological representations, in that they omit many details that we see and that would appear in an image, such as shape and path direction, leaving behind only an irreducible meaning, such as path itself. Image-schemas are a format for storing accessible meanings and cannot themselves be brought to awareness. From early on, infants attend to beginning and endings of object paths. e.g. if a moving object stops before touching another, even if the gap is very small [Leslie 82; leslie/keeble 1987] 3-months differentiate bilogically correct from incorrect motion of both people and animals [Arterberry/Bornstein 2001] [Bertenthal 1993] This discrimination may originally be a kind of automatic perceptual schematizing, but at some point through attentive processing it forms a conceptual package associated w self-motion. 9-mo olds distressed on seeing a mechanical robot start to move on its own [Poulin-Dubois, Lepage, Ferland 1996] --> violation of expectations abt inanim behaviour [Spelke Philips 1995] 7-mo : look longer at inanim objects that self-start Evidence that inanim / anim distinction may be based on motion rather than any shape / appearance attribute. 5- to 9-mo olds attention is focused on end-point or goal of a reach. By 9 months, infants differentiate between intentionally grasping an object vs accidentally dropping a hand on it. [Woodward:1998, 1999] 10-11-mo olds - learned something abt structures of actions. videos in which everyday actions were interrupted/suspended in the middle or end were looked at longer. [Baldwin, Baird Saylor Clark 2001] ***IDEA for expt [Gergely Csibra etal - 1999, 1995] use computer displays showing geom forms moving around and interacting in various ways. by end of 1st yr, infants clearly distinguish goal-directed paths from unmotivated trajectories. Displays consist of moving circles, so there was no figural info at all. [Johnson Sockaci 2000] 14-mo olds treat purple blobs as agents if they engage in goal-directed activity. [Johnson Slaughter Carey 1998] when amorphous object has face w eyes or had no face but acted contingently w infants, if it turned towards an object, 12-mo olds would follow its "gaze" Arterberry, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2001). Three-month-old infants’ categorization of animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 333–346. Baldwin,D. A., Baird, J. A., Saylor,M. M., & Clark, M.A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action. Child Development, 72, 708–717. Bertenthal, B. (1993). Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints. In C. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Bíró, S., Koós, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267. Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Vol. 2. Language, thought, and culture (pp. 301–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193. Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., & Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233–238. Johnson, S. C., & Sockaci, E. (2000, July). The categorization of agents from actions. Poster presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Brighton, England. Leslie,A.M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173–186. Leslie, A., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265–288. Poulin-Dubois, D., Lepage, A., & Ferland, D. (1996). Infants’ concept of animacy. Cognitive Development, 11, 19–36. Poulin-Dubois, D., & Vyncke, J. (2003). The cow jumped over the moon: Infants’ inductive generalization of motion properties. Concordia University, Montreal: Unpublished manuscript. Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action. In A. J. Premack, D. Premack, & D. Sperber, (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 44–77). Oxford: Clarendon Press. Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49–100. Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach.Cognition, 69, 1–34. Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145–160.
(Three experimental studies) [Gentner 1982] : across several lgs, children's early productive vocabs appear to be dominated by nouns. [Gentner/Boroditsky:2001] this is not because "nouns are easy", but because the conceptual basis for nouns are more likely to be "preindividuated" - distinguished from others, more likely to have become categories. Cross-linguistic comparative data:. English, Italian, Spanish: favour nouns Mandarin, and perhaps Korean, : do not prefer Ns because Ns are not as frequent in the input in these lgs. Also, children use each of their verbs more frequently than the Ns, so spontaneous speech samples (e.g. 1hr) tend to underestim Ns. Better methodology may be vocab checklist. In [Tardif etal 1999] on Mandarin, the V advantage almost disappeared in the vocab checklist.
author={Goksun, T. and Hirsh-Pasek, K. and Golinkoff, R.M.}, pages={33--42}, IN (shop cart to kid seat) monies.IN (looking for coins between cushions) from Melissa. M. Bowerman 1999 - Learning to structure space for language: a cross-linguistic perspective in Language and space: edited by Paul Bloom, M.A.Peterson, Nadel and Garett; 1999, MIT Press IN.( shop cart to kid-seat) Monies.IN ( looking for coins dropped b/w cushions) Books. OUT Books.BACK ( books taken out from and put back into a box) Monkey.UP ( on seeing monkey ON TV jump onto a couch) DOWN.Drop ( toy falls off couch ) ON. ( cellotape stuck on chair ) OFF ( pushing mom's hand off her drawing paper) OPEN mommy ( wants adult to straighten bent "mommy" doll ) spatial conceptual structures is available from about 6 mos old; once lg comes, how are these mapped Order of acquisition: In -> On -> Under why is this the ordering? is it because of some motivation or urge to communicate locational states? that they forget phonemes not native to the lg is not quite correct but the forgetting of tight fitting in korean etc is not quite analogical - there is no perceptual reorganization and loss japanese: "go" has many manner variants; "go through" is turu, "go under" is different etc. --- Abstract Relational terms (e.g., verbs and prepositions) are the cornerstone of language development, bringing together two distinct fields: linguistic theory and infants’ event processing. To acquire relational terms such as run, walk, in, and on, infants must first perceive and conceptualize components of dynamic events such as containment–support, path–manner, source–goal, and figure–ground. Infants must then uncover how the particular language they are learning encodes these constructs. This review addresses the interaction of language learning with infants’ conceptualization of these nonlinguistic spatial event components. We present the thesis that infants start with language-general nonlinguistic constructs that are gradually refined and tuned to the requirements of their native language. In effect, infants are trading spaces, maintaining their sensitivity to some relational distinctions while dampening other distinctions, depending on how their native language expresses these constructs. --- Infants might learn relational language in a similar fashion (for similar arguments see also Choi, 2006; Clark, 2003, 2004; Hespos & Spelke, 2007). They might notice a common set of foundational components of events regardless of the language they are learning. Then, influenced by distinctions encoded in the native language, they might focus on a subset of these components that are relevant to their native language. Analogously, this phenomenon might be called semantic reorganization, in which universal perceptual constructs are reorganized to match the expressional tendencies of one’s native tongue. Language, in this case, would have the function of orienting infants’ attention to some relations in events over others. --- The time for action is now. It’s never too late to do something. —Antoine de Saint-Exupery This is a world of action, and not for moping and droning in. —Charles Dickens When I was kidnapped, my parents snapped into action. They rented out my room. —Woody Allen
Introduction: Progress on the Verb Learning Front 3 Roberta Michnick Golinkoff and Kathy Hirsh-Pasek
1 Finding the Verbs: Distributional Cues to Categories Available to Young Learners 31 Toben H. Mintz 2 Finding Verb Forms Within the Continuous Speech Stream 64 Thierry Nazzi and Derek Houston 3 Discovering Verbs Through Multiple-Cue Integration 88 Morten H. Christiansen and Padraic Monaghan
4 Actions Organize the Infant’s World 111 Jean M. Mandler 5 Conceptual Foundations for Verb Learning: Celebrating the Event 134 Rachel Pulverman, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Roberta M. Golinkoff, Shannon Pruden, and Sara J. Salkind 6 Precursors to Verb Learning: Infants’ Understanding of Motion Events 160 Marianella Casasola, Jui Bhagwat, and Kim T. Ferguson 7 Preverbal Spatial Cognition and Language-Specific Input: Categories of Containment and Support 191 Soonja Choi 8 The Roots of Verbs in Prelinguistic Action Knowledge 208 Jennifer Sootsman Buresh, Amanda Woodward, and Camille W. Brune 9 When Is a Grasp a Grasp? Characterizing Some Basic Components of Human Action Processing 228 Jeffrey T. Loucks and Dare Baldwin 10 Word, Intention, and Action: A Two-Tiered Model of Action Word Learning 262 Diane Poulin-Dubois and James N. Forbes 11 Verbs, Actions, and Intentions 286 Douglas A. Behrend and Jason Scofield
12 Are Nouns Easier to Learn Than Verbs? Three Experimental Studies 311 Jane B. Childers and Michael Tomasello 13 Verbs at the Very Beginning: Parallels Between Comprehension and Input 336 Letitia R. Naigles and Erika Hoff 14 A Unified Theory of Word Learning: Putting Verb Acquisition in Context 364 Mandy J. Maguire, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, and Roberta Michnick Golinkoff 15 Who’s the Subject? Sentence Structure and Verb Meaning 392 Cynthia Fisher and Hyun-joo Song
16 Verb Learning as a Probe Into Children’s Grammars 429 Jeffrey Lidz 17 Revisiting the Noun-Verb Debate: A Cross-Linguistic Comparison of Novel Noun and Verb Learning in English-, Japanese-, and Chinese-Speaking Children 450 Mutsumi Imai, Etsuko Haryu, Hiroyuki Okada, Li Lianjing, and Jun Shigematsu 18 But Are They Really Verbs? Chinese Words for Action 477 Twila Tardif 19 Influences of Object Knowledge on the Acquisition of Verbs in English and Japanese 499 Alan W. Kersten, Linda B. Smith, and Hanako Yoshida 20 East and West: A Role for Culture in the Acquisition of Nouns and Verbs 525 Tracy A. Lavin, D. Geoffrey Hall, and Sandra R. Waxman 21 Why Verbs Are Hard to Learn 544 Dedre Gentner