Keith, Kenneth D. (ed);
Cross-Cultural Psychology: Contemporary Themes and Perspectives
John Wiley & Sons, 2011, 600 pages
ISBN 1444351796, 9781444351798
topics: | psychology | social | culture |
I found myself … on a high hill … With me was a Pygmy youth, named Kenge … Kenge was then about 22 yr. old, and had never before seen a view such as this … Kenge looked over the plains and down to where a herd of about a hundred buffalo were grazing some miles away. He asked me what kind of insects they were, and I told him they were buffalo, twice as big as the forest buffalo known to him. He laughed loudly and told me not to tell such stupid stories. (Turnbull, 1961, pp. 304–305)
Early work by W. H. R. Rivers (1864–1922), neurologist and anthropologist: was the first to systematically study cross-cultural perception. On the people of Torres Straits, between northeastern Australia and New Guinea... inhabitants of Murray Island (whose language only has words for the colors black, white, and red) typically grouped green and blue tiles together. These findings were typical for other cultures (and languages) being studied at the time, and prompted the Whorf Hypothesis, which explicitly states that language determines our experience. However, research has not fully supported this hypothesis. Rosch (1973) examined the color discrimination of the Dani tribe of Papua, New Guinea, and determined that although the language only has two terms (one for all dark colors and one for all light colors), the Dani were able to discriminate several colors from one another. This occurred even though, when given a sorting task, members of the Dani tribe typically sorted the color tiles into two groups. Davies and Corbett (1997) examined color sortings from native speakers of English, Russian, and Setswana. Though Russian has two words for blue (one for light blue and one for dark blue), the sortings from English and Russian speakers were very similar. The Setswana speakers, however, tended to sort the blues and greens together, and they used only one word for these colors. It seems, then, that language does not drive perception, but perhaps the importance of having particular words for different colors in a language is dependent upon the need for communicating those colors among individuals.
German psychiatrist Franz Müller-Lyer created this illusion in 1889 (Bermond & Van Heerden, 1996). W.H.R. Rivers sought to determine the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on three groups of Murray Islanders: men, boys, and girls. At the time it was believed that the Murray Islanders (being less "civilized") would be more susceptible to the illusion (that is, make greater errors). Participants were given a brass slide with convergent arrowheads (set at a standard length of 75 mm, and depicted as segment ‘A’) that contained an inner slide with a divergent arrowhead at one end. Rivers tested the three groups of Murray Islanders (men, boys, and girls) and compared their performance to three groups of English participants (students, adults, and schoolchildren). Interestingly, the Murray Islanders performed better than their English counterparts (they more accurately slid the guide so that the line segments had equal length). Subjects have to slide the right half until segments A and B have equal length. Murray islanders did better than Europeans.
Segall, Campbell, and Herskovits (1966) tested more than 1,300 people in 17 different groups, including children (aged 6–11) from 12 of the groups. Most of the participants (more than 1,000) were from 10 different African countries, one small sample comprised Europeans in South Africa, another small sample included Hanunóo people in the Philippines, and 264 individuals were included in two American samples. Segall et al. found substantial differences among the groups. Data were reported based on the percentage greater line AB had to be before it was judged equal to line CD. Kalahari Bushmen and adult workers in South African gold mines showed virtually no effect of the illusion at all (their error sensitivity was 1%, meaning that they correctly judged when the two line segments were of equal length). The groups most susceptible to the illusion were the two American samples: a group of university students and children and adults from Illinois. This pattern of results clearly demonstrated differences between groups. Moreover, the results also showed that children (regardless of group) were more susceptible to the illusion than adults, with the proportional difference between children and adults for any cultural group being fairly systematic (the correlation between children's and adults’ ratings for all groups was 0.81). Segall et al. (1966) proposed the "carpentered-world hypothesis," which states that children who grow up and live in squared, city-block environments and rectangular buildings are more susceptible to the illusion (Figure 8.3). How could one determine whether the differences reported by Segall et al. (1966) are due to culture (specifically, a carpentered world) or due to genetics? (It is apparent that the groups studied by Segall differed genetically.) Pedersen and Wheeler (1983) did the next best thing: they tested 20 members of the Navajo Indian tribe, 10 of whom lived until at least age 6 in a hogan (the typical Navajo rounded house), and 10 of whom lived in a rectangular house. The results were consistent with predictions from the carpentered-world hypothesis—those Navajo students reared in rectangular houses were more susceptible to the Müller-Lyer illusion than those raised in the hogan. Other studies incorporating Chinese participants have also shown support for the carpentered-world hypothesis (Dawson, Young, & Choi, 1973).
ce suggests that Western (industrialized) cultures have far greater three-dimensional perception, and many African subjects interpret ostensibly 3D-images as flat. Thus, they are more easily able to copy an image of the trident, since it doesn't confuse them. This suggests that some aspects of perception are not innate, but acquired based on cultural exposure.
Anthropologist William Hudson showed pictures like this one: Hudson asked children and adults questions such as "What is the man doing?". many native Africans would say he was about to spear the elephant, ignoring the depth cues. Hudson also observed that pre-school western children also tended to make similar judgments. Hudson suggested that 'habitual exposure to pictures play a large role' in pictorial perception.
[However, Hudson's practices in collecting data have been challenged, see JM Kennedy's searing critique of Hudson's methods in his Psychology of Picture Perception, Chapter 5: It is not surprising that many of Hudson's subjects seemed to base their replies to his question on logical argument, avoiding relying on weak perceptual cues. For example, some subjects argued that a man would not attack an elephant with a spear, so if the man in a picture was throwing a spear, it must be at the antelope, not the elephant in the picture. Some subjects told Hudson, flatly, that the pictures were ambiguous. If he wanted to question them about the pictures, the subjects said, he should tell them which view they should take. Hudson did not discuss the sophistication of his subjects with tests. He did not say whether his subjects were all equally at ease. But surely when a white man pulls a black laborer away from his daily work and sits down in an office with the laborer, and begins to show the laborer little pictures, the laborer begins to feel a little anxious. Especially when the setting is South Africa, the laborer must be uncomfortable. To make matters worse, the white man waits unhelpfully through long periods without deigning to assist the laborer in answering the odd questions the white man is asking. Hudson reports that at times the response was given by thesubject after a lag of one hour! What fears were in the laborers' minds we can only guess at. p.73