Dunlea, Anne;
Vision and the emergence of meaning: blind and sighted children's early language
Cambridge University Press, 1989, 196 pages [gbook]
ISBN 0521304962, 9780521304962
topics: | cognitive | blind | developmental | psychology | language |
The ways into language seem to rely heavily on exploiting the non-linguistic and immediate environment, and on utilizing visually based strategies for gaining and directing attention. ix `% Visual information has been [related to] many facets of the process of language acquisition as it normally progresses. ... thought to be important in fostering early parent-infant interaction, in providing the child with a stimulus for hypothesizing about what language encodes, and in supplying the parents with clues about what a young child's early verbalizations mean. Furthermore, vision seems to be crucial in the infant's conceptualization of the environment, on which early language development is thought to depend.
Stern (1974, 1977) points out that the infant's first exposure to the human world is composed of his mother's activities, especially her repertoire of "infant elicited behaviors." These center on exaggerated facial expressions, accompanied by vocalizations and gazing at the infant. The human neonate has a strong propensity to observe and even imitate these expressions (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977), with the result that they form the core of interactive play. The play episodes themselves are typically initiated by caregivers using a combination of eye gaze and vocalization in which the objective is to obtain mutual orientation in a face-to-face position with the infant (Stern, 1977; Tronick et ai, 1979; Kaye, 1979). Once the infant is attending, a play-dialogue ensues until the infant disengages by glancing away. The best predictor of when a mother will respond again to the infant is the moment that the infant's gaze again focuses on her (Brazelton et aL, 1974; Stern, 1974; Fogel, 1977). Thus, visual attention on the part of both the caregiver and the infant is crucial in initiating and maintaining early exchanges. These rudimentary exchanges are structured along the same lines as the adult discourse system... Without access to visual information, the structure of these interactions is necessarily disrupted and there seems to be no substitute for their effectiveness in establishing a bond between parents and infants, and in initiating the human infant into the social world from which language emerges (Fraiberg, 1977). As the child begins to use language, visual information seems to provide an important stimulus for building hypotheses about meaning. For example, in ascribing meaning to words, the child appears to abstract certain salient attributes from early referents and uses these as a basis for extending the domain of application for words (Bowerman, 1976, 1978; Clark, 1973; Nelson, 1973a; Rescorla, 1980). This process is essential in helping the child move from using a word as a "name" for a specific referent, to using words as symbolic vehicles to denote a heterogeneous class of referents.
The overwhelming evidence is that such visual properties as shape, size, and movement are the most important criteria used in constructing these classifications. Not only is visual information important in the child's organization of referent properties, but it appears to underlie adult categorization and the structure of many lexical fields as well (Andersen, 1978; Clark, 1977b; Rosch, 1975, 1977). Some of the evidence for this comes from the analysis of classifier systems in a variety of natural languages. Classifiers are expressions which group together entities that share some particular attribute. English does not exploit these, though the principle can be seen in the utterance "She bought four lengths of material." Some languages classify all varieties of countable objects yielding such sentences as "She has seven round-things eggs." Clark's (1977b) analysis of classifier systems reveals that perceptual information, again largely visual, is the primary basis of groupings. The features round, long and flat are especially important. For example, the Indonesian language groups such objects as fruit, peas, eyes, balls, and stones together on the basis of roundness; Nung groups together trees, bamboo, thread, nails, and candles on the basis of length; Kachari groups together leaves, fans, and cloth on the basis of flatness. Even in cultures which do not have classifier systems, these features are important. As the result of a number of experiments conducted in a variety of cultures, Rosch (1973, 1975, 1977) has found that people tend to group objects on the basis of perceptual features, especially the visual perception of shape... defining lexical classes from infancy on, and visual information seems to be central in this. ... The Piagetian notion of interaction with the environment as the basis of sensorimotor intelligence specifically involves perception, especially visual attention to objects and events, as well as purely motoric behavior.... It appears that the course of development is hampered for infants who are born blind (Piaget and Inhelder, 1969). This is significant for the present discussion, since language acquisition seems to depend on the emerging conceptual system and the young child's developmental task involves matching linguistic and cognitive structures (Clark, 1977a; Nelson, 1974; Pylyshyn, 1977). One area where this is particularly evident is in the child's early expression of semantic roles in which such fundamental relations as Agent + Object or Agent + Action are thought to reflect the child's understanding of and experience with his immediate environment. Perhaps the most frequent explanation for how very young children come to understand and produce language is that they depend on the "here and now," which has been defined as "whatever is directly under the child's eyes" (Clark and Clark, 1977, p. 322). The child learns about the matching of language and world largely through context.
A now classic example is Shatz's (1974) analysis of how toddlers successfully respond to such directives as "Can you shut the door?" ... the child follows his parent's eye gaze and gesture which are directed toward the door, a strategy which crucially depends on vision. The child's previous observations and explorations equip him with the knowledge that doors can be opened and closed, and the child may pick up the parent's intonation and recognize the utterance as a directive. p.4 Perception of distant objects (sun stars) large objects (mountain rivers) etc. is more difficult. No evidence that senses are finer - tactile, hearing are both about the same. However, senses are used more effectively, e.g. echolocation - bright blind children spontaneously learn to clap and echolocate around age 1.5-2. 10 congenitally blind, if vision is restored as adult by surgery, have difficulty recognizing objects visually - need training. [Molyneux problem] 11 Blind infants - not much work on testing them - devising elicitations is more difficult. 11 First vocalizations: relatively stable sound pattern in particular situation. Differs from pre-lg babbling in that they are stable and repr a consistent communicative function - but not actually words - may disagree with adult usage. Diary studies indicate that these patterns originate in their imitating various environmental sounds, incl their own spontaneous noises (Guillaume 78, Leopold 1939,1949;
object concept related to ear-hand coordination (similar to eye-hand in sighted child). reaching for objects is a bit delayed - in sighted child around 4-5 mos, in the blind, averaging 9 mos (6.5 to 11 mos). [Warren 70, Bower 74] [Blind children do not appear to have a clear distinction between spatial deictives like here and there, this and that. also slower to learn spatial concepts]
Fraiberg (1977) used the Bayley Scales to document language acquisition in ten blind subjects who were participating in her developmental/psychoanalytic study of blindness in children. The Bayley items record only the very broadest achievements (e.g., "says two words") and are limited to the very earliest aspects of language. The results suggest language development in blind children progresses at a normal pace, though different skills may have ontogenetically differing significance for the two groups. For example, Fraiberg's subjects are precocious in their attainment of the items "jabbers expressively" and "imitates words" while they are delayed in "says two words" and "sentences of two words". The first differences may indicate the importance of acoustic signals to blind children, whereas later acquisition of words and word combinations is in line with the reported delay in object concept for blind children. Language acquisition in blind children - point to differences: - Urwin (1978a), - Rowland (1980), - Mulford (1980, 1986), - Kekelis (1981), Mills (1983b), - Landau and Gleitman (1985), - Wilson (1985) and Bigelow (1987). Urwin's pioneering study reveals that nursery rhymes incorporating body actions, such as clapping or rocking, provide an unusually effective means for parents to gain and maintain their blind infant's attention. These routines may then become the basis of social play and provide a means for the blind children to initiate and control interaction during their second year. Interestingly, nursery routines became less important for one subject Urwin studied who had some residual vision, a pattern more typical of sighted children. Language itself seemed to evolve out of these rituals for her subjects; a number of early words and phrases derived from familiar routines. Nursery routines may thus provide an alternative to the visually based pointing, gesturing and offering games that evolve in interactions with sighted children and that seem to facilitate language (see Ninio and Bruner, 1978; Masur, 1982). The impact of verbal routines may in fact be quite pervasive. Urwin suggests that the beginnings of representational play in her blind subjects were not in games with objects, but in reconstructing conversations. Beginning at 1.5 years, one subject actually used different voice qualities to mark different people's speech: for example, a gruff voice for his father and a lowvoicefor his mother. The important point here isthat blind children may use language to begin to engage in role-play and possibly to represent themselves. This contrasts with Fraiberg's (1977) view that self representation (and correct use of pronouns) evolves much later in blind children and may be associated with object play. Urwin also found that a rapid expansion in vocabulary at about 18 months occurred concurrently for her children with the emergence of representational play. This play involved objects for the child with residual vision but mainly verbal role play for the totally blind children. However, ... a close examination of roleplay speech reveals some difficulties blind children have in understanding reversibility of perspective and deictic terms (Andersen, Dunlea and Kekelis, 1984). Just as verbal role-play may be characteristic of blind children, there is also a tendency for them to engage in a considerable amount of sound and word play not directed to others and there is a striking tendency to reproduce segments of speech that sound like their caregiver's language (Urwin, 1978a; Wilson, 1985; Fraiberg, 1977). This characteristic is what some clinicians call "delayed echolalia" but it is important to distinguish between children whose language is almost exclusively echoic and children who use language in many situations including echoically. Only the former is regarded as pathological. Moreover, sighted children also pick up and use unanalyzed chunks of language which they eventually dissect (Peters, 1977, 1983). ... Mothers appear to compensate for their infant's visual impairment by using greater physical contact and vocalizing to respond not only to their infant's vocalizations, but also to their smiles and other behaviors (Urwin, 1978a; Rowland, 1980, 1984). Language inevitably serves an important function in maintaining contact. Speech directed to blind children may be more centered on the children themselves than on their activities with objects (Urwin, 1978a; Kekelis, 1981; Andersen and Kekelis, 1982). In comparison with sighted children, blind children seem to receive fewer statements describing activities and events and more labels for objects or requests for the child to identify objects. Kekelis suggests that this is because it is more difficult to direct blind children's attention outside themselves and to monitor their attention to events. In general, she suggests these strategies appear to encourage visually impaired children to take an active role in conversations, but they may also limit the kinds of information provided to these children. After a possible mild delay in beginning to say words, blind children seem to acquire 50 or so words in the usual amount of time reported for sighted children - a few months - then, as their vocabularies expand, they begin to say two- and three-word phrases. Several researchers looking at the early stages of language development have found blind and sighted children's language fairly similar (Bigelow, 1986; Landau and Gleitman, 1985). The strongest claim comes from Landau and Gleitman who argue that "the blind learner can surmount whatever obstacles diminution of experience places in her path, acquiring her native tongue in a largely unexceptional fashion" (p. viii). They later suggest that blind and sighted children are linguistically indistinguishable by 36 months. Such claims are based on surveying the content ofearly vocabularies and the semantic roles expressed in early multiword phrases and by calculating mean length of utterances.2 A number of researchers have found the content of blind children's early vocabularies fairly similar to sighted children's when classified along the lines proposed by Nelson (1973a) (Bigelow, 1986; Landau and Gleitman, 1985; Urwin, 1978a). In combining data from a number of sources, including some unpublished data from the present study, Mulford (1986) found the most notable differences were a relatively high proportion of specific nominals and action words in the blind children's vocabularies, a lower proportion of general nominals and surprisingly few function words (e.g. "more," "what," "all gone"). There are fewer studies of blind children's early multi-word language. Landau and Gleitman report that the three children they studied expressed similar types of semantic roles to the sighted children studied by Bloom, Lightbown and Hood (1975). [But] the vague category "other" is relatively large for the blind children, leading the reader to wonder if some interesting differences remain undiscovered. (Landau and Gleitman, p. 37) Urwin: classified the two-word combinations produced by her oldest subject according to Brown's (1973) semantic categories. While all of the categories were attested, locative relationships were rarely expressed and the utterances often referred to the child's own activities. As we shall see later, this hints at an important thematic difference in the way blind and sighted children use language. The greatest delay reported at the multi-word level is Landau and Gleitman's finding that their blind subjects were late in acquiring auxialiary verbs. They attribute this to a proportionally greater number of declaratives in mothers' language to their blind children and a correspondingly fewer number of yes-no questions. In English, auxiliaries are stressed in yes-no questions and are therefore relatively salient in these structures, which would presumably aid children in learning them. other areas in which blind children's language development is seen as different from sighted children's. In particular, Mills' discussion of phonology (1983a) and Mulford's work on reference (1980, 1983) suggest some difficulties. Mulford's investigation of referential terms in three to five year old blind children indicates that pronoun use appears to be the same for her blind subjects as for the sighted five year olds described by Wales (1979). However, she did find the blind children making semantically based errors. * exophoric pronominal references, those which relate to experience and the situational context, were less frequent than endophoric references for the blind children. * Endophoric references relate to language and the linguistic context, and may therefore be more accessible to the blind. Perhaps most interesting, Mulford found that the blind children used deictic terms such as "this" and "there" as names for locations and entities but were not sensitive to the relative value of deictics with respect to the speaker. Mulford's analysis demonstrates the importance of delving beneath the surface to examine qualitative differences in use that may be obscured by simple counts of arguments of the verb and the deictic referents that co-occur with these interjectives. It would seem that this subject is using an inferential strategy based on syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic information and not simply the syntactic strategy Landau and Gleitman propose.
The pioneering work of Clark (1973, 1975) and Nelson (1973b) assumes that children build word meanings on the basis ofone or two stable elements ofmeaning. Clark's theory ofsemantic feature acquisition, based on analysis of numerous diary records, proposes that children begin by associating only one or two features with a word and gradually add additional criterial features until the child's understanding of the term corresponds to adult usage. The child's first hypotheses about what words mean stem from the conceptual organization of non-linguistic information, particularly the perceptually based features of shape, movement, size, texture, sound, and taste. For any given word, children initially command a limited number of features and may overextend the word to cover other referents which share one or more of the features. The following example based on shape is illustrative: Lexical item: mooi (child acquiring English) First referent: moon Domain of application: cakes, round marks on windows, and in books, round shapes in books, tooling on leather book covers, round post marks, letter 'O' Note: based on Clark (1973), p. 80 There are also instances of "partial overextension" where a term is extended to new referents which have just one of two dominant features (e.g., the features of movement and roughness associated with a toy goat are the basis of one child's extending his name for that toy to all moving objects and also to all rough surfaces). Although organizational strategies are thought to underlie early lexical development, many, psychologists hold that young children are incapable of classifying (see Inhelder and Piaget, 1964; Vygotsky, 1962), while others hold that intensive classification does originate during the sensorimotor period (Cohen and Strauss, 1979). Nelson's investigation of spontaneous sorting in infants between the ages of 12and 24 months indicates that children engage in consistent sorting and grouping activities prior to the acquisition ofrelevant language and that the sorting criteria are generally functional in nature, that is, they are based on the child's understanding of how an item is used and of the dynamic states of objects. In this view, objects are initially viewed in terms of their "functional cores" from the child's perspective. Subsequent researchers have interpreted Nelson's position to be that lexical meaning is based on the extension of this functional information (see Barrett, 1978; Gentner, 1978; Press, 1974; Thomson and Chapman, 1977). In fact, Nelson proposed that while functional criteria are initially defining, extensions may also proceed on the basis of perceptual criteria.
The notion that children build word meanings on the basis of one or two stable elements of meaning contrasts with the more traditional view in psychology that early word meanings are used as "complexive" groupings. That is, a word is extended on the basis of some recognized similarity between two referents, then may be extended to a third referent on the basis ofsome feature shared with a preceding referent, but the various referents do not all share the same features. Vygotsky's (1962) classic example is ofa child who extends the word "quah" from a duck swimming in a pond, to all liquids, then to a coin with an eagle on it, and finally to all round coin-like objects. In fact, there are reports of both categorical and complexive uses of early words and neither strategy seems to have ontogenetic priority over the other. An alternative to the feature component model of lexical development draws on the prototype model of category structure (see Rosch, 1973, 1975a, 1977; Rosch and Mervis, 1975). The crucial insight is that categories for both adults and children may consist of a core meaning, or a focal exemplar, which is surrounded by other category members of progressively decreasing similarity to the core meaning. Category boundaries are by definition vague, since peripheral members may exhibit a fairly high degree of fluidity: they may shift category membership in response to circumstances prevailing at a given time. A good exemplar of the category "bird" is a robin, while a penguin isa more peripheral member. An implication of this theory for child development is that rather than focusing only on the child's acquisition of criterial attributes, researchers should also consider the role of core meaning in children's initial lexical classifications. Application of the theory reveals that the complexive strategies used in early categorization are not necessarily the primitive "chained complexes" proposed by Vygotsky that reflect unstable categories, but rather that category membership isa matter ofdegree ofvariation from a prototype (see Bowerman, 1976, 1977, 1978, 1979 and for slightly older children Andersen, 1975 and Anglin, 1977). All referents share something, but not necessarily the same thing, with a prototype referent. This is of course similar to Clark's notion of partial extension.