Dunbar, Robin;
Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language
Harvard University Press 1997-03-10 (Hardcover, 242 pages
ISBN 9780674363342 / 0674363345 (pacificBookXch 08jun)
topics: | evolution | language | science | anthropology
About our propensity for gossip, Dunbar asks:
Why on earth is so much time devoted by so many to the discussion of so little?
He then takes an evolutionary perspective and outlines how during evolution, social bonds strengthened, so that apes and monkeys share larger and more intense, close-knit social relationships than other animals. The grooming among primates is more about this social bond (and status) than about hygiene.
As the social group became even larger (as in a human village), it was no longer possible to sustain N choose 2 grooming relations, and it became necessary to find a more convenient form of multi-person "grooming". This, Dunbar argues, is gossip. Language, he says evolved more as an enabling force for this gossip, rather than on its natural selection merits alone.
Very well written - almost lyrical in snatches.
To be groomed by a monkey is to experience primordial enotions: The initial frisson of uncertainty in an untested relationship, the gradual surrender to another's avid fingers flickering expertly across bare skin, the light pinching and picking and nibbling of flesh as hands of discovery move in surprise from one freckle to another newly discovered mole. - (opening lines, p. 1)
A human baby produces its first real words at about 18 months of age.
2 years - quite vocal; vocab of some 50 words. 3 years - 1000 - 2/3 word sentences 6 years - 13,000 words 18 yrs - 60K p.3 Our brain is nine times larger rel to body size than a mammal. Only porpoises and dolphins come close. Most human conversations - 2/3ds is about social issues p.4 [no serious research data]. newspapers: L. Times: 1993 col-inches; 43% column space (850 in) goes to "human interest stories" - rest to serious news /reviews; human interest = interviews, gossip etc. Sun: 1063 col-in; 78% human interest, 833in (only 22% serious news) Great Ape extinction - from 30mya to ~10mya, more apes than monkeys; but 10mya, temp drop by 10 deg C (ocean surface) - and most ape species die. [possibly because apes can't handle higher tannin diet as in unripe fruit]
Andrew Whiten and Dick Byrne : chacma baboons in S. Africa. Young adult female (Mel) was digging a succulent tuber out of the ground... hard work... Young juvenile (Paul) was watching. As soon as Mel managed to wrench the tuber out of the ground, Paul let out an ear-splitting scream, of the kind commonly uttered by juveniles who are being attacked by someone bigger. Paul's mother, who was beyond some bushes, immediately came racing, took in the situation at a glance, and fell on the unsuspecting Mel. Needless to say, Mel dropped the tuber and ran off, with outraged mum in pursuit. Paul nonchalantly picked up the tuber and started to enjoy lunch. 23 lg: "high dudgeon" - can dudgeon ever be "low"? (now used only in the phrase `in high dudgeon' - freedictionary: thesaurus) Reconciliation after fighting among male Geladea baboons: [After a fight, often lasting over a whole day, to decide who gets the harem. The females may also help decide such battles, if they prefer one over the other. ] The new male approaches the defeated male in a non-aggressive, almost submissive way. The defeated male is initially suspicious. He has just received the beating of his life from this thug... After some false starts, the deal is struck surprisingly quickly; there is a simple ritual of reconciliation in which the old male reaches through to touch the new male's penis while the latter presents his rear. Then the two males groom each other for the kind of enthusiasm reservved for the aftermath of patched-up quarrels. 27 Grooming stimulates production of endorphins - the body's natural opiates. Opium etc. work because their chemical signature is identical to that of the more familiar opiate drugs such as opium or its derivative, morphine. However we don't become addicted to endogenous opiates in quite the way we do to opium because the brain only produces natural opiates in relatively small quantities. [or is it because we have no "control" over this natural production?] Stress makes it more difficult to conceive (studies on career women) - also men who ran > 60 miles / week had sign lower sperm densities p.43 - this is perhaps why lower-ranked female geladas, who face some degree of harrassment from other higher females - 2 mild threats / day and one serious attack per week - they may have higher stress, which is why they produce fewer offspring - appox 10% less for every drop in rank. Also lower ranking females have higher levels of circulating endogenous opiates. [May be crucial also in explaining some aspects of behaviour - such as some days when everything seems to go well - one wins in sports, does well at work, etc. Low opiates? ] Free riders in social situations Magnus Enquist / Otto Leimar : percentage of free riders - those who promise to return a benefit but don't - rises as size of group gets bigger. Because the free-rider can keep one step ahead of being "discovered". In many species, the female will only mate w males who bring them gifts. Making an expensive investment beforehand forces the male to invest enough in the female, and not to abandon her. 46 Charles Hockett, 1960s: 18 features which can be taken to define true language. 4 most important are that language is: - referential - sounds refer to objects in environment - syntactical (has grammatical structure) - non-iconic - most words do not resemble objects they refer to (unlike the "word" moo for example) - learned (as opp to being instinctive) were proposed to distinguish human language from e.g. language of honey bees, discovered in the 1950s by ethologist Karl von Frisch. Through a combination of ingenuous experiments, was able to show that the speed of the dance indicates distance to the nectar source, while angle of the bar of the figure of eight to the vertical indicates the compass direction rel to the sun. [from http://www.cals.ncsu.edu/entomology/apiculture/PDF%20files/1.11.pdf] When a food source is very close to the hive (less than 50 meters), a forager performs a round dance. She does so by running around in narrow circles, suddenly reversing direction to her original course. She may repeat the dance several times at the same location or move to another location on the comb to repeat it. After the round dance has ended, she often distributes food to the bees following her. A round dance, therefore, communicates distance ("close to the hive," in this example), but not direction. Food sources that are at intermediate distances, between 50 and 150 meters from the hive, are described by the sickle dance. This dance is crescent-shaped and represents a transitional dance between the round dance and a waggle dance. The waggle dance or wag-tail dance, is performed by bees foraging at food sources that are more than 150 meters from the hive. This dance, unlike the round dance, communicates both distance and direction. A bee that performs a waggle dance runs straight ahead for a short distance, returns in a semicircle to the starting point, runs again through the straight course, then makes a semicircle in the opposite direction to complete a full figure-eight circuit. While running the straight-line course of the dance, the bee's body, especially the abdomen, wags vigorously from side to side. This vibration of the body produces a tail-wagging motion. At the same time, the bee emits a buzzing sound, produced by wingbeats at a low audio frequency of 250 to 300 hertz or cycles per second. The buzzing occurs in pulsebeats of about 20 milliseconds, delivered at a rate of about 30 per second. While several variables of the waggle dance relate to distance (such as dance "tempo" or the duration of buzzing sounds), the duration of the straight-run portion of the dance, measured in seconds, is the simplest and most reliable indicator of distance. As the distance to the food source increases, the duration of the waggling portion of the dance (the "waggle run") also increases. The relationship is roughly linear (Figure 3). For example, a forager that performs a waggle run that lasts 2.5 seconds is recruiting for a food source located about 2,625 meters away. The orientation of the dancing bee during the straight portion of her waggle dance indicates the location of the food source relative to the sun. The angle that the bee adopts, relative to vertical, represents the angle to the flowers relative to the direction of the sun outside the hive. In other words, the dancing bee transposes the solar angle into the gravitational angle.
1950s: Kellogg family and Hayes family - raised chimpanzee baby along with own new-born infant. Hayes' chimp - Vicki - managed to produce ~ 5 words, but barely audibly. Worse, Hayes' own child picked up innumerable bad habits from the faster-maturing chimp, and they gave up. 51 late 50s: became clear that chimps did not have the vocal apparatus for speech. 1960s: Trixie Gardner and husband Alan: sign language to young female chimp Washoe. taught ASL. Learned ~100 signs, became the "wonder of the age". But some psychologists / linguists argued that Washoe demonstrated nothing more than the ability to copy humans, as in the "Clever Hans" phenomenon. Rarely produced sentences > 2 signs long, (excluding repetitions). The supposedly novel signs she produced, e.g. water+bird on seeing a swan - were no more than chance combinations fueled by the Gardners' imaginative interpretation. Gardners spent next 20 years trying to refute these charges, but opponents devised ever more ingenuous arguments. 1970s: ASL w gorilla Koko and orangutan Chantek picture-language (to avoid imitation arguments] with chimp Sarah - psychologist David Premack. Duane Rumbaugh - two chimps - Austin and Sherman - taught a computer keyboard language called "yerkish" (after psy pioneer Robert Yerkes) Sue-Savage Rumbaugh: Kanzi - taught same computer lg. "became the Einstien and Shakespeare of the chimpanzee world, rolled into one." Dunbar, however, isn't careful enough in detailing some of his stronger remarks like the following - I think it is fair to say this research has convincingly demonstrated that chimps understand several important concepts, including numbers, how to add and subtract, the nature of basic relations (such as "is bigger than", "is the same as" and "is on top of"), how to ask for specific objects (mostly foods) or activities (a walk in the woods or a game of chase), and how to carry out complex instructions ("take the can from the fridge and put it in the next room"). Kanzi can translate readily from one modality to another: e.g. by pointing to the correct keyboard symbols for spoken Engl words heard through headphones. However, even Kanzi's interactions are largely limited to asking for things he wants, or giving the correct one-word answer to logically complex questions. He does not engage in the kind of spontaneous, apparently effortless chatter of the two-year-old human child learning to speak: "Look, mummy, car!" .... "Yes, dear, another car..." 54 Often the logic is unclear - e.g. arguing how fruit-based diet justifies the relatively larger brain/body-size ratio in monkeys. 59 (see Jerison based species graph, 57)
Poem by computer program called RACTER, by NY programmer Bill Chamberlain: A hot and torrid bloom which Fans wise flames and begs to be Redeemed by forces black and strong Will now oppose my naked will And force me into regions of despair. published as the slim volume The Policeman's Beard is Half Constructed in 1984. [snippets of prose interspersed with poetry and a strange short story about a dysfunctional group of friends sitting down for a dinner of lamb chops. - complete text
Why is it that among all the primates, only humans have language? Could it be because we don't groom each other? Dunbar builds his argument touching on the behavior of gelada baboons, Darwin's theory of evolution, computer-generated poetry, and the significance of brain size. He begins with the social organization of the great apes. These animals live in small groups and maintain social cohesion through almost constant grooming activities. Grooming is a way to forge alliances, establish hierarchy, offer comfort, or make apology. Once a population expands beyond a certain number, however, it becomes impossible for each member to maintain constant physical contact with every other member of the group. Considering the large groups in which human beings have found it necessary to live, Dunbar posits that we developed language as a substitute for physical intimacy. Whether or not you accept Dunbar's premise, his book is worth reading, if only for its animated prose and wealth of scientific information. An obvious choice for science buffs, Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language is a wonderful book for anyone with an inquiring mind and an interest in what makes the world go round.
What a big brain we have for all the small talk we make. It's an evolutionary riddle that at long last makes sense in this intriguing book about what gossip has done for our talkative species. Psychologist Robin Dunbar looks at gossip as an instrument of social order and cohesion--much like the endless grooming with which our primate cousins tend to their social relationships. Apes and monkeys, humanity's closest kin, differ from other animals in the intensity of these relationships. All their grooming is not so much about hygiene as it is about cementing bonds, making friends, and influencing fellow primates. But for early humans, grooming as a way to social success posed a problem: given their large social groups of 150 or so, our earliest ancestors would have had to spend almost half their time grooming one another--an impossible burden. What Dunbar suggests--and his research, whether in the realm of primatology or in that of gossip, confirms--is that humans developed language to serve the same purpose, but far more efficiently. It seems there is nothing idle about chatter, which holds together a diverse, dynamic group--whether of hunter-gatherers, soldiers, or workmates. Anthropologists have long assumed that language developed in relationships among males during activities such as hunting. Dunbar's original and extremely interesting studies suggest otherwise: that language in fact evolved in response to our need to keep up to date with friends and family. We needed conversation to stay in touch, and we still need it in ways that will not be satisfied by teleconferencing, email, or any other communication technology. As Dunbar shows, the impersonal world of cyberspace will not fulfill our primordial need for face-to-face contact. From the nit-picking of chimpanzees to our chats at coffee break, from neuroscience to paleoanthropology, Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language offers a provocative view of what makes us human, what holds us together, and what sets us apart.
to contribute some excerpts from your favourite book to
book
excerptise. send us a plain text file with
page-numbered extracts from your favourite book. You can preface your
extracts with a short review.
email to (bookexcerptise [at] gmail [dot] com).
We reply to all feedback!